[ home ] |
PSYC3006
Cultural Psychology and the Media
Lecture 17: Social construction and
the media effects debate
© Dave Hiles, March 2004
(1) The psychology of media effects
Media effects is the
idea that the media, in all of its many forms, has an effect on human
behaviour. On how we think, on how we see the world, on how we see
ourselves, etc. It is a research topic within social psychology with a
fairly long history - one that has recently been heavily criticized. For
example, the social constructionist perspective would stress the
discursive rather than the causal aspects of media images, i.e. the mean
making processes involved.
Task: At
the start of this lecture, let us think of some of the possible
ways in which the "Media" may be thought to influence
our behaviour. |
Here are some possible responses:
- anti drugs campaigns
- food
"panics" - eggs, BSE
- AIDS awareness
- contraception, road
safety, etc
- political/economic agendas
- exposure to images of violence, sex, etc
- images of attractiveness,
ideal body image, normality, etc
- benign exposure to fashion, style, (publicity, hyping, etc)
- censorship
of the INTERNET
- entertainment (ie. it was
Goebbels who agued that the most powerful form of propaganda was
ENTERTAINMENT).
In this lecture we will focus on the issue of media effects with
particular reference to images of violence, which has been the
contentious issue within film studies.
(2) Violence and the mass media
see Gauntlett (1995; 1998)
Stated briefly:
The media effects
model: representation of violence in the mass media violence
causes violence in society |
Some issues raised by Gauntlett:
- the desensitization hypothesis
- the ‘zombie’ hypothesis
- cultivation effects
- new directions in effects research
- panic vs. programmes
(3) Empirical
research of mass media and violence
There has been a long history of the empirical study of
"media violence":
Laboratory studies - Feshbach (1955; 1961); Berkowitz (1963);
Bandura (1962; 1965); Borden (1975).
Field studies - Feshbach & Singer (1971); Leyens et al
(1975); Parke et al (1977).
Natural/found experiments - Hennigan et al (1982); Milavsky
(1988); Messner (1986).
Correlational studies -
Longitudinal panel studies -
Meta-analysis - Paik & Comstock (1994).
(4) The Critical
Perspective
"One of the reasons we have made so little progress in our
mitigating of violence is that we have determinedly overlooked the
elements in it that are attractive, alluring and fascinating." Rollo
May (1972, p.165)
Some observations:
- there is a large body of
criticism of effects research
- why the focus on violence?
- moral panics, scapegoating - the long tradition
- the focus on visual representation
- the
eschewing of context
- the
focus on children
- Barker
& Petley’s (1997) comments on Newson (1995):
how does
common sense operate?
how can we tell the message of a film?
how can we understand media influence?
what exactly is media violence? |
Theoretical issues:
- No consideration of a Social
Constructionist approach
- Postmodernist issues
- A Cultural Psychology perspective and a discursive approach
Joseph Klapper's (1960) approach to the typical role of the media in
effecting change
It is worth noting Klapper’s early proposals for not taking an
over-simplistic view of media effects:
(1) the mass media are not normally enough to produce change - they
contribute in association with other influences and factors;
(2) normally, the role of mass communication is that of reinforcing
the status quo - maintaining behaviours, values, beliefs;
(3) when the mass media produce change rather than reinforcement, it
is social factors that become active towards change, or become
inoperative in their reinforcing role;
(4) the degree of change is effected by the nature of the medium, the
persuasive message, the social climate, etc.
David Gauntlett’s criticism of the effects model
Gauntlett has been particularly critical of the media effects
debate, and is especially critical of the research conducted by
psychologists. He has recently summed up his position as follows:
"Media ‘effects’
studies - by which we mean those studies which seek to identify a
particular 'effect' which is the result of exposure to a
particular type of media content - have had a most unusual history
.The majority of this research has been centred, predictably
enough, on the question of whether watching violence on screen
will lead individuals to be aggressive in real life. On the one
hand these studies have been quite popular, with newspapers and
politicians always eager to have more of them, and with several
(usually American) academics always eager to build entire careers
on producing them. On the other hand they have been entirely
useless, showing nothing except the somewhat interesting fact of
their own redundancy.
"The central problem for these studies is that
isolating one particular thing, such as TV viewing or magazine
reading, as the cause of a person's behaviour is basically
impossible. The idea that a bit of media content ‘'made’
somebody do something will always seem silly, for the perfectly
good reason that, as we all know, the influences upon any decision
to do something are a complex combination of many elements,
including previous experiences, opinions, values and suggestions
from various sources. It might seem overhasty to dismiss these
studies out of hand. Instead one could consider each piece of
research in some detail, as I tried to do once in a whole book
dissecting these studies (Moving Experiences, 1995). But to
do that is really to take these studies much too seriously. Their
individual flaws were often curious, amusing and a bit depressing,
but would not usually be worthy of much attention, were it not for
the fact that ‘media effects’ continues to be a subject of
much public discussion - and also because some ‘experts’ like
to cheekily claim that the case for negative media effects has
been proved. (On a UK television discussion about screen violence
in 1994, for example, American psychologist Leonard Eron
confidently told the audience that in the USA this was no longer
an issue for debate: ‘The search for media effects is over,’
he declared, asserting that ‘conclusive proof’ had finally
persuaded everyone that media content could have a clear and
identifiable influence on people’s behaviour. This claim was, of
course, not true). For this reason it remains important to be able
to look at the studies themselves - to show, for what it's worth,
that they don't show anything" (Gauntlett, 2002, p. 28-29). |
David Gauntlett (1998) provides a hard-hitting critique of ‘Ten
things wrong with the effects model,’ with particular relation to
violence and children. This summary
comes from Burton (1999), to read the full paper: http://www.theory.org.uk/effects.htm
- The effects model tackles social problems backwards. It does
not start with the social violence which is the point of reference and
then work towards media usage.
- The effects model treats children as inadequate. Largely as a
result of assumptions made from within theories of psychological
development, it is assumed that children do not or cannot ‘cope’
with media violence. Research that deals with children’s responses
suggests otherwise.
- Assumptions within the effects model are characterised by barely
concealed conservative ideology. Most noise about media violence
is made by commentators who demonstrably start from a conservative
position on the potential of media for encouraging violence.
- The effects model inadequately defines its own objects of study. Definitions
of what is anti-social or violent behaviour in media are sometimes
partial and highly questionable.
- The effects model is often based on artificial studies. The
methodology of study - eg the artificiality of laboratory experiments
- may undermine the conclusions.
- The effects model is often based on studies with misapplied
methodologies. Inconsistencies and false correlations are cited by
Gauntlett as examples.
- The effects model is selective in its criticisms of media
depictions of violence. Research is dominated by attention to
fiction, rather than looking at news and violence, for example.
- The effects model assumes superiority to the masses. Just as
people interviewed about media effects tend to deny these, so also
researchers themselves take a superior, detached attitude to effects
on themselves and how these may influence their work.
- The effects model makes no attempt to understand meanings of the
media. Assumptions about a clear message from the media, as well
as about the detached ability of the researchers to identify that
message, ignore the complexity of meanings produced and then adduced
by media users.
- The effects model is not grounded in theory. Assumptions
about effects are simplistic and too often founded on assertions.
Contexts, reasons for media behaviour, and other questions about what
is going on are largely ignored.
(5) Empirical vs. Discursive research
Rachel Shaw (2000) offers this
comparison:
|
Empirical Approach |
Discursive Approach |
Aim |
To
establish a causal law linking viewing violence in the media with
violent behaviour in society |
To
understand the experience of violence per se; watching media
violence and encounters with violence in real life. |
Method |
To
observe behaviour through experiments, field studies, longitudinal
studies, surveys, etc. |
To
discursively analyse accounts of people’s experiences of
violence. |
Assumptions |
That
watching media violence is harmful and may result in societal
decline. |
That
violent films provide entertainment and can be a source of
pleasure: paradoxical to real life violence. That the experience
of violence is contextualised. |
Findings |
The
Effects Discourse: watching media violence can result in increased
aggression and therefore increased violent behaviour. |
The
Existential Needs Discourse: watching media violence satisfies an
existential need safely in fantasy rather than in reality. |
Why media
violence? |
Increased
use of media violence represents a society lacking in moral
judgment that believes violence is an appropriate solution to life’s
problems. Violence is viewed as a negative component of human
existence that should be eradicated. |
Violence
has endured through the centuries perhaps demonstrating an
intrinsic human fascination with violence. Film and television
violence is the latest medium through which violence can become
entertainment. Violence is viewed as a positive expression of
emotions in which humankind finds some kind of pleasure and
satisfaction |
(6) Viewing violence in context
Annette Hill's (1997) study found that:
- The process of viewing violence is a self-conscious activity.
- The viewing environment signifies an awareness of context.
- Viewers gauge their own response to violence by monitoring the
response of others.
- Audience awareness is closely linked with physical and emotional
responses.
- Anticipation when viewing violence is a key factor in the range of
response. Shared anticipation and enjoying the challenge of
anticipation play an important role.
- Laughing at violence is recognised as different to other laughter.
- There is awareness that men and women react differently to
representations of violence.
Men Viewing Violence (1998) Broadcasting Standards
Commisssion.
This involved a study of men watching soap opera, television drama,
documentary, feature film, sport. With respect to feature films (Basic
Instinct, Under Siege) broadcast on television, the issue of textual
context is raised:
The violence was
" . . neither seen as believable nor as affecting but as a
device to liven up the action."
"One of the films had been edited for television
broadcast. For these respondents, such censorship was unnecessary
because neither the violence the violence nor its context was seen
as believable."
|
[ Top of Page ]
[ home ]
|